性别绑定的语言影响THE GENDERLINKED LANGUAGE EFFECT.doc
《性别绑定的语言影响THE GENDERLINKED LANGUAGE EFFECT.doc》由会员分享,可在线阅读,更多相关《性别绑定的语言影响THE GENDERLINKED LANGUAGE EFFECT.doc(4页珍藏版)》请在三一办公上搜索。
1、THE GENDER-LINKED LANGUAGE EFFECT Anthony Mulac, University of California, Santa Barbara1. What is the Gender-Linked Language Effect?In a substantial number of empirical investigations outside the context of organizations, the language used by men and women has been shown to differ in meaningful way
2、s. Mulac, Bradac, and Gibbons (in press) summarized more than 30 studies, finding 16 language features that have differentiated gender in a consistent manor. For example, 5 investigations have shown that men tend to use more references to quantity (“an 81% loss in vision”) than women, and 3 studies
3、have revealed that men employ more judgmental adjectives (“Working can be a drag”). On the other hand, 6 studies have demonstrated that women use more intensive adverbs (“This is really hard”) and 5 that women use more references to emotions (“If he loved what he was doing . . .”). Although such lan
4、guage differences are often found, they should not be thought of as “markers” of gender (Giles, Scherer, & Taylor, 1979) whose presence unerringly points to the gender of the speaker. Instead, they function as gender-linked “tendencies” (Smith, 1985) to favor certain linguistic features over others.
5、 Although there is widespread agreement among researchers that gender-linked language differences occur in a wide range of communication contexts (Aries, 1996; Henley & Kramarae, 1991; Pearson, West, & Turner, 1993), a challenge to this view has recently appeared. Canary and Hause (1993) have argued
6、 that meaningful differences in the communication strategies of men and women have not been found with any degree of consistency. They conclude, “We believe there are sex differences in communication, but they are eluding us” (p. 141). Unfortunately, Canary and Hause cite only 3 of the more than 30
7、empirical studies summarized by Mulac et al. (1998) that have found gender differences in language use in a wide variety of contexts. The importance of these gender-linked tendencies can be seen in the effects of such language differences on observers judgments of communicators. In a series of eight
8、 investigations, Mulac and his colleagues have demonstrated that mens and womens language leads them to be judged differently on psychological dimensions that are of consequence (cf. Mulac & Bradac, 1995; Mulac & Lundell, 1980, 1982, 1986, 1994). The almost universal finding is that readers of brief
9、 transcripts of womens language rate them higher on Socio-Intellectual Status (i.e., higher social status and more literate) and higher on Aesthetic Quality (more pleasant and beautiful). Men are rated higher on Dynamism (stronger and more aggressive). This pattern of judgments has been shown, by mu
10、ltiple regression analyses, to be linked to gender-differentiating language and has been titled the Gender-Linked Language Effect (see Mulac, 1998, for a complete summary). In these studies, the researchers have employed transcripts of male and female communication recorded in a variety of contexts:
11、 public speeches, oral descriptions of landscape photographs, written essays on morality, written descriptions of photographs, and problem-solving dyadic interactions between strangers. Although many of the studies have involved university students as speakers, writers, or dyad partners, a substanti
12、al number have used communicators of other ages: fourth- and fifth-grade students, university teaching assistants and lecturers older than 30, and people in their 50s and 60s. The pattern of results has been essentially identical across all communicator age-groups, although one study showed the effe
13、ct to be greater for older speakers (Mulac & Lundell, 1980). () In all of their investigations, Mulac and associates have controlled for the possibility that gender stereotypes might affect ratings by ensuring that observers were unable to identify the sex of the speakers or writers. They reasoned t
14、hat if observers could not identify the sex of the communicators, they could not be influenced by gender stereotypes when they rated those communicators. However, in another investigation, Mulac, Incontro, and James (1985) directly compared the effects of male and female language differences to thos
15、e of gender stereotypes. Results showed that observers made remarkably similar judgments about men and women, based on either the speakers language use or the observers own gender stereotypical notions about men and women (86% judgment overlap). Furthermore, the findings indicated that language and
16、stereotype effects are independent of each other, in that they can be brought about separately, added together to increase male-female differences, or pitted against each other to cancel out such differences. One possible interpretation is that the way in which men and women speak helps perpetuate g
17、ender stereotypes. These findings of evaluative consequences of male/female language differences have been found equally for male and female raters across the eight investigations (Mulac, 1998). In addition, three of these studies found that older individuals (median age of more than 40 years) provi
18、ded speaker ratings that were essentially identical to those of university students (median age = 19). The consistency of these findings serves to substantiate the broad generalizability of the Gender-Linked Language Effect. 2. Descriptions, Examples, and Citations for 18 Language Features Found in
19、Previous Empirical Studies to Predict Communicator Gender1. SENTENCESA. Elliptical sentences (“Gorgeous!” “A beautiful snowy setting.” “Daytime.”):A unit beginning with a capital letter and ending with a period in which either the subject or predicate is understood. Mulac and Lundell (1986),M+b (ora
20、l descriptions of photographs); Mulac and Lundell (1994), M+ (written descriptions of photographs).B. Questions (“What is Communication 12?” “What do you do?”): Directives in question form were not counted. Fishman (1978), F+ (couples conversations); Mulac, Wiemann, Widenmann, and Gibson (1988), F+
21、(dyadic interactions). C. Directives (“Think of another.” “Why dont we put that down?”): Haas (1979), M+ (interviews); Mulac et al. (1988), M+ (dyadic interactions). D. Negations (“You dont feel like looking . . .”): A statement of what something is not. Mulac and Lundell (1986), F+ (oral descriptio
22、ns of photographs); Mulac, Lundell, and Bradac (1986), F+ (public speeches). E. Mean length sentences: The number of words divided by the number of sentences, defined as sequences of words beginning with a capital letter and ending with a period. Hunt (1965), F+ (written essays); Mulac et al. (1986)
- 配套讲稿:
如PPT文件的首页显示word图标,表示该PPT已包含配套word讲稿。双击word图标可打开word文档。
- 特殊限制:
部分文档作品中含有的国旗、国徽等图片,仅作为作品整体效果示例展示,禁止商用。设计者仅对作品中独创性部分享有著作权。
- 关 键 词:
- 性别绑定的语言影响THE GENDERLINKED LANGUAGE EFFECT 性别 绑定 语言 影响 THE

链接地址:https://www.31ppt.com/p-3029460.html